The Art of Choosing a Romantic Partner (Part One)

Why do we choose the romantic partners that we do?  And what shapes the choices we make?   While scientists have been weighing those question for decades (and philosophers have been doing the same for centuries), nobody has managed to come up with a clear answer yet – but not for want of trying.  

According to evolutionary psychologists, for example, humans are seen as primarily attracted to sex partners with specific characteristics that will boost reproductive success. This means that females seek out potential partners who bear primary and secondary sex characteristics they regard as signs of genetic fitness (deep voices, masculine characteristics, indicators that they are good providers etc.) while men seek to make themselves as attractive as possible through competition with other males.   

But that’s just one perspective regarding human mating behaviour. According to the concept of assortative mating, we are most likely to be attracted to partners with whom we share basic similarities. These similarities can take the form of genetic characteristics (e..g., research showing that men prefer females whose faces are similar to their own) or can involve similarities in cultural, ethnic, or socioeconomic factors.  Then again, if you don’ t happen to agree with evolutionary or assortative theories relating to mating, there is always the self-deception model of romantic behaviour.  

According to this model, lasting relationships are formed when people develop positive illusions that make them less likely to recognize the flaws in their romantic partners.  Much like William Blake’s poem, “Love to faults is always blind/Always is to joy inclin’d/Lawless, wing’d, and unconfin’d/And breaks all chains from every mind,” cultivating positive illusions can help individuals turn a blind eye to their partner’s flaws. In other words, looking at potential partners through rose-coloured glasses can help ensure domestic bliss.  

But most people won’t just settle on one partner. As we grow and mature, we tend to acquire multiple romantic partners over the course of an average lifetime. Which then brings us to how consistent we are in our mating choices.  Do the kind of partners we select as teenagers match up with the partners we might choose when we are older and, presumably, wiser?   

Given the confusing variety of different theories about human mating, the obvious question arises over  whether it’s even possible to make sense of what is ultimately a highly personal and, at times, irrational decision. It’s hardly a cliche for many parents to be confused about the romantic choices their children make, not to mention other friends or family members.  Even as we grow as individuals, we may find ourselves making different choices in terms of who we might choose to be with.  But is it really possible for researchers to understand matters of the heart?   

A new research article published in the Journal of Personality and Social Psychology attempts to answer this particular question by examining how consistent people really are in the romantic partners they choose. Paul W. Eastwick of the University of California Davis and a team of co-researchers conducted a series of studies focusing on the different factors that go into romantic pairings, particularly in terms of those stable factors that influence the romantic choices we make.  These are the long-term factors that can cause people to choose similar partners over time.   

According to Eastwick and his colleagues, the stable factors  that seem to dominate mate selection ares :

  • active stable factors –  these are the qualities that individuals use to evaluate potential partners and by which they  in turn are evaluated. Whether it involves physical attractiveness, appealing personality traits such as humour or compassion, intelligence, or evidence of being a good provider, these different factors can help people judge whether a potential partner has  high or low value as a mate.  Many people may not even be consciously aware that they tend to choose in specific ways but similarity-attraction effects often leads to “clustering” among current and former partners due to how much they share specific characteristics. Another stable factor that often plays an important role is relationship aptitude. This deals with how effective people can be in forming and maintaining new relationships  depending on their prior relationship experience and  personality.  
  • passive stable factors stem from the social environment in which we all live which can also shape mating choices. Overall, we are most likely to meet and date people whom we might encounter on a frequent basis. This includes dating someone who lives in the same neighbourhood, goes to the same school, works in the same building (or nearby), etc.  Also known as propinquity, it can be a powerful influence in the relationships we choose.   People are also more likely to become involved with those who are similar to them in some way, whether in terms of belonging to the same ethnic group, religious congregation, or having the same financial background, etc.  

In their research, Eastwick et al. conducted three empirical studies and one simulation study looking at the similarities that occur among current and former partners and how active and passive factors can influence mating choices. The simulation study was used to confirm that people selecting mates based on specific qualities would tend to show clustering among current and former partners on those particular qualities even when the number of mates being selected is very small.   But how well does this apply in real-life relationships?

With the first empirical study, 136 undergraduate students provided Internet links to photographs of two or more current or ex-partners which were then analysed in terms of  similarities that were visible in the images used. Participants were told they were completing a survey on how people depict themselves on social media. They were then asked to provide links to Facebook profile pictures of people with whom they were currently involved sexually or had been involved with in the past. Participants also indicated for each partner whether he or she was (a) a “current boyfriend/girlfriend” (N = 45), (b) an “ex/former boyfriend/girlfriend” (N = 128), (c) a “current non-committed partner (e.g., hookup, friend with benefits)” (N = 24), or (d) a “past/former non-committed partner (e.g., hookup, friend with benefits)” (N = 231). On average, each participant provided links to 4.4 partners and provided over 428 photographs for the study.  

All of the photographs were then rated on three dimensions:  attractiveness, masculinity/femininity, and dominance and were then compared to photographs of the participants themselves. Results showed significant clustering along these traits for both past and present romantic partners as well in comparison to each of the participants. In other words, people tend to choose partners who are similar to themselves in many ways and also tend to be consistent in terms of the type of partner they prefer.

Since the participants and the partners they provided photographs for tended to fall within a very narrow age range, Eastwick and his colleagues conducted a second study to see if their results hold up in a much broader group.  Using data from Add Health, a long-term study of adolescent health, 303 men and 271 women were recruited who each nominated two or more romantic partners for whom self-report data was available. A total of 1,110 different partners were examined over the course of the study  (same-sex relationships were excluded for the purpose of the study),   

Among the Add Health variables that were examined in the study were:   educational aspirations, depression, intelligence, self-esteem, vitality, delinquent behaviour, and religiosity.  Demographic factors such as parental income, parental education, and partner racial background were also included. Results showed evidence for partner clustering among the different variables studied though the overall effect size was small. In fact, most of the clustering seemed to occur based on passive stable factors, i.e., people with similar backgrounds or who lived relatively close to one another were more likely to form relationships.

But how well does the evidence for clustering in the kind of romantic partners we may choose hold up over time?  More on that next week.

To be continued

http://ift.tt/2toW7rk

Advertisements

Leave a Reply

Fill in your details below or click an icon to log in:

WordPress.com Logo

You are commenting using your WordPress.com account. Log Out / Change )

Twitter picture

You are commenting using your Twitter account. Log Out / Change )

Facebook photo

You are commenting using your Facebook account. Log Out / Change )

Google+ photo

You are commenting using your Google+ account. Log Out / Change )

Connecting to %s